Tipultech logo

Goal contents theory

Author: Dr Simon Moss

Overview

According to goal contents theory, one of the five principles of self-determination theory (Vansteenkiste, Niemiec, & Soenens, 2010)--some of the goals that individuals pursue are more likely to promote wellbeing than other goals (Kasser & Ryan, 1996). In particular, some goals--such as endeavors that relate to community support, personal growth, and the formation of close relationships--are called intrinsic (Kasser & Ryan, 1996). These goals foster autonomy, competence, and relatedness, three of the fundamental needs of individuals (see need fulfillment). Consequently, these goals seem enjoyable, challenging, fulfilling, and important, sometimes called intrinsic motivation. These motivations tend to enhance persistence and improve wellbeing.

In contrast, some goals that individuals pursue seem to be extrinsic, relating to accumulating wealth and improving reputation. These goals often impede autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Consequently, these extrinsic goals impair wellbeing (Kasser & Ryan, 1996) and learning (Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004).

Intrinsic goals, although related to intrinsic or autonomous motivation, are nevertheless distinct. For example, an individual can act altruistically, an intrinsic goal, merely to impress a colleague, an extrinsic motivation or reason. Hence, goals and motivations can be distinct. Indeed, intrinsic goals are related to wellbeing even after the reasons or motives to pursue these goals--and thus intrinsic or autonomous motivation--are controlled (Sheldon, Ryan, Deci, & Kasser, 2004).

Mechanisms that underpin the benefits of intrinsic goals

Several explanations can reconcile the relationship between intrinsic goals and wellbeing. Intrinsic goals, by definition, facilitate the achievement of basic psychological needs. Accordingly, these goals are more inclined to promote wellbeing (Deci & Ryan, 2000). In contrast, extrinsic goals do not fulfill these basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness, and hence the satisfaction that is derived from their achievement are transient (Deci & Ryan, 2000).

Several explanations can also accommodate the observed relationship between intrinsic goals and learning. Specifically, in a learning context, individuals who pursue extrinsic rather than intrinsic goals will not become engaged in all facets of the endeavor, but will restrict their efforts to the activities they regard as instrument to some tangible outcome (Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Soenens, & Matos, 2005). Learning is impaired and satisfaction diminishes, because core needs are not fulfilled.

Consequences of extrinsic goals and intrinsic goals

Response to intergroup threats

People who pursue extrinsic goals may feel more threatened by other groups, such as ethnic minorities, than people who pursue intrinsic goals. That is, according to Duriez, Meeus, and Vansteenkiste (2012), individuals who value extrinsic goals are especially concerned with money and status. They may feel that other groups could secure jobs they desire, diminishing their prospects to earn more money. They may also feel that minority groups could threaten their status as the dominant social category. Consequently, these individuals may be especially concerned about the ascent of minority groups.

In contrast, people who value intrinsic goals are not as concerned about money or status. Instead, they value relationships, community, and personal development. Minorities may not impede these goals and indeed could even facilitate the pursuit of these objectives, especially personal growth and contribution to the community.

Duriez, Meeus, and Vansteenkiste (2012) conducted a study that vindicates this possibility. In this study, participants completed the aspiration index, developed and validated by Kasser and Ryan (1996). To gauge the extent to which they pursue extrinsic goals, participants were asked to indicate the importance they attach to financial success, image, and fame. Other questions gauged the degree to which individuals attach importance to intrinsic goals, including growth, contribution to the community, and affiliation.

In addition, participants, all of whom were Belgian, read an article that indicated that Poland plans to join the European Union. Some but not all participants were informed of the adverse consequences that could unfold if Poland did join the European Union. For example, they were informed that unemployment and crime could skyrocket. Finally, participants were asked to indicate the degree to which they perceived the possibility that Poland would join the EU as very enriching or threatening.

If participants tended to pursue extrinsic goals, they were very likely to perceive this decision of Poland to join the European Union as very threatening, especially if the adverse consequences of this possibility were emphasized. In contrast, if participants tended to pursue intrinsic goals, they were not as threatened by this possibility.

Measures of extrinsic and intrinsic goals

The aspiration index, constructed and validated by Kasser and Ryan (1996), is often utilized to measure the degree to which individuals value extrinsic goals and intrinsic goals. Specifically, to gauge the extent to which they pursue extrinsic goals, participants are asked to indicate the importance they attach to financial success (e.g., "It is important for me to be financially successful in life"), image (e.g., "It is important for me to be attractive and good-looking"), and fame (e.g., "It is important for me to receive recognition and admiration for the things I do"). To gauge the degree to which they pursue intrinsic goals, participants indicate the importance they attach to growth (e.g., "It is important for me to develop myself and continue to grow as a person"), contribution to the community (e.g., "It is important for me to try to do things that improve society"), and affiliation (e.g., "It is important for me to build solid and intimate friendships?").

Duriez, Meeus, and Vansteenkiste (2012) showed these items actually load on one factor. The extrinsic goals load positively on this factor and the intrinsic goals load negatively on this factor. All loadings exceed .50. Consequently, researchers tend to reverse score the intrinsic goals. The aggregate reflects the extent to which participants value extrinsic goals over intrinsic goals. Cronbach's alpha was .78

Resolution of identity and intimacy stages

According to Erikson, throughout their life, people need to resolve various crises, each of which surfaces during specific life stages. The successful resolution of each crisis or conflict will uncover competencies and capabilities that can be helpful to the future.

For example, as individuals transcend adolescence and begin the path of adulthood, they need to either clarify their identity or continue to experience confusion about their role in life. For example, they need to resolve questions like "Who am I" and "Where do I fit in the world"? After people resolve this crisis, they tend to agree with statements like "The important things in life are clear to me".

During early adulthood, individuals often reach another crisis in which they need to decide whether to embrace the possibility of rejection, and seek a deep, open, caring, and committed relationship, or to remain isolated. If they embrace intimacy, they agree with statements like "I care deeply for others" but not "Bgfeing alone with other people makes me feel uncomfortable".

Hope, Milyavskaya, Holding, and Koestner (2013) proposed and validated the argument that a shift towards more intrinsic goals, such as the goal to improve society, extend knowledge, or develop relationships, facilitates the resolution of these two crises. If individuals embrace these intrinsic goals, they experience a sense of autonomy. This sense of autonomy enables people to clarify their values& their identity is thus certain. Likewise, this clarity may increase the resilience of individuals, because they strive to sustain their pursuits regardless of obstacles. Because of this resilience, individuals may be willing to experience the rejection that perhaps the pursuit of intimacy can entail.

Consistent with these possibilities, participants who become more likely to embrace intrinsic goals over time were more likely to experience clarity in identity and intimacy in relationships. In addition, clarify of identity, but not intimacy, was positively associated with psychological wellbeing.

Determinants of extrinsic goals and intrinsic goals

Awe

When individuals experience a sense of awe, they are more inclined to choose experiences over material goods. In particular, as emphasized by Rudd, Vohs, and Aaker (2012), if people are exposed to something that is vast and remarkable, they feel a sense of awe and became entranced or immersed in this feat or event. This immersion in their immediate environment increases the likelihood that people do not feel as rushed as usual. Consequently, they feel they are granted the time to embrace experiences.

To illustrate, in one study, conducted by Rudd, Vohs, and Aaker (2012), to evoke a sense of awe, some participants imagined ascending the Eiffel Tower and seeing Paris from this height. Other participants imagined ascending an unnamed tower and seeing a plain landscape. Next, they were asked to indicate the extent to which they felt rushed as well as whether they would prefer to spend money on experiences, such as a Broadway show, or material goods, such as a watch. If awe was evoked, participants were more likely to prefer the experiences. This relationship was mediated by the sense that time is sufficient rather than rushed and restricted.

Reflections on internal values

As Lekes, Hope, Gouvei, Koestner, and Philippe (2012) showed, if individuals are inspired to reflect upon their intrinsic values, such as relationship development, personal growth, or community service, their values actually change. They become more likely to treasure these intrinsic values, and their wellbeing improves accordingly.

This possibility was demonstrated by Lekes, Hope, Gouvei, Koestner, and Philippe (2012). In their study, the participants were university students. In one condition, participants first read about the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic values as well as the discovery that intrinsic values enhance wellbeing. Next, they received a list of 13 intrinsic values. They were asked to choose the two values that are most significant to them. For 15 to 20 minutes, they wrote about why these values are important to them and how they manifest or incorporate these values in daily life. In the control condition, participants completed a similar exercise, but instead wrote about daily activities instead of intrinsic values. After this exercise, they completed various indices of wellbeing, a measure that gauges their values, and a shortened version of the social desirability index.

Over the next four weeks, participants were prompted each week to reflect upon these intrinsic values or activities again. The extent to which they felt engaged in this activity was gauged. Finally, all the measures were administered after four weeks.

Compared to participants who wrote about daily activities, participants who wrote about intrinsic values reported greater wellbeing--including positive emotions and vitality--immediately. Then, over the next four weeks, they also reported greater levels of intrinsic motivation and wellbeing, provided they felt engaged in this procedure. These findings persisted even after social desirability was controlled.

Factors that moderate the consequences of extrinsic goals and intrinsic goals

Caprariello and Reis (2012) challenge the proposition that life experiences per se are more likely to enhance happiness than are material possessions. Instead, they believe that any activities that promote social interaction and sharing tend to foster happiness. Life experiences may, in general, be more beneficial than a material acquisition merely because they foster social interaction and sharing. Indeed, as they showed, after social interactions and sharing were controlled, the benefits of life experiences over material possessions were no longer significant.

For example, in one study, on each trial, individuals needed to decide which of two purchases they prefer. In general, they preferred social experiences to material purchases in general as well as material purchases in general to solitary experiences. In a later study, participants reflected upon a time in which they bought one of four purchases using discretionary income: a solitary life experience, a life experience with at least one other person, a solitary material possession, or a material possession that promotes social interactions, such as a game. In general, social purchases, regardless of whether they were life experiences or material possessions, were more likely to elicit feelings of happiness.

Several mechanisms could explain the benefits of social experiences. For example, people are more likely to converse about social experiences, and these discussions tend to amplify the experience.

In some circumstances, intrinsic goals may not enhance life satisfaction or happiness. Within a Peruvian slum, for example, intrinsic goals were inversely associated with wellbeing, as shown by Guillen-Royo and Kasser (2015). Presumably, when unachievable goals are salient, wellbeing tends to diminish, at least transiently.

References

Adelberg, S., & Batson, C. D. (1978). Accountability and helping: When needs exceed resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 343-350.

Amabile, T. M., DeJong, W., & Lepper, M. (1976). Effects of externally imposed deadlines onsubsequent intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 92-98.

Ambrose, M. L., Arnaud, A., & Schminke, M. (2008). Individual moral development and ethical climate: The influence of person-organization fit on job attitudes. Journal of Business Ethics, 77, 323-333.

Amos, E. A., & Weathington, B. L. (2008). An analysis of the relation between employee-organization value congruence and employee attitudes. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 142, 615-631.

Ariely, D. (2009). Predictably irrational: The hidden forces that shape our decisions. London: Harper.

Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

Burgess, M., Enzle, M. E., & Schmaltz, R. (2004). Defeating the potentially deleterious effects of externally imposed deadlines. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 868-877.

Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 64, 363-423.

Caprariello, P. A., & Reis, H. T. (2012). To do, to have, or to share? Valuing experiences over material possessions depends on the involvement of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 199-215. doi: 10.1037/a0030953

Carbonneau, N., Vallerand, R. J., & Massicotte, S. (2010). Is the practice of yoga associated with positive outcomes? The role of passion. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 452-465.

Carnevale, P., Pruitt, D., & Seilheimer, S. (1981). Looking and competing: Accountability and visual access in integrative bargaining. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 111-120.

Chang, L., Shih, C., & Lin, S. (2010). The mediating role of psychological empowerment on job satisfaction and organizational commitment for school health nurses: A cross-sectional questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 47, 427-433.

Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1988). The empowerment process: integrating theory and practice. Academy of Management Review, 13, 471-482.

De Cuyper, N., & De Witte, H. (2009). Volition and reasons for accepting temporary employment. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 17, 363-387.

Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115.

Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237-288). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for true self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 31-49). New York: Plenum Press.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and the "why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being across life's domains. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 49, 14-23.

Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119-142.

Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627-668.

Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagne, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern Bloc country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 930-942.

Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M. (1981). An instrument to assess adults' orientations toward control versus autonomy with children: Reflections on intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 642-650. 

Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

Duriez, B., Meeus, J., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2012). Why are some people more susceptible to ingroup threat than others? The importance of a relative extrinsic to intrinsic value orientation. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 164-172. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.003

Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth? American Psychologist, 51, 1153-1166.

Fernet, C., Guay, F., & Senecal, C. (2004). Adjusting to job demands: The role of work self-determination and job control in predicting burnout. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 39-56.

Filak, V. & Sheldon, K. M. (2003). Student psychological need-satisfaction and college teacher-course evaluations. Educational Psychology, 23, 235-247.

Filak, V. & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Teacher support, student motivation, student need satisfaction, and college teacher course evaluations: Testing a sequential path model. Educational Psychology, 28, 711-724.

Frey, B. S. (1997). Not just for the money: An economic theory of personal motivation. Cheltenham, UK: Elgar.

Frey, B. S. (2001). Inspiring economics: Human motivation in political economy. Cheltenham, UK: Elgar.

Frey, B. S., & Jegen, R. (2000). Motivational crowding theory: A survey of empirical evidence. Journal of Economic Surveys, 5, 589-611

Gagne, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199- 223.

Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. W. (2011). The necessity of others is the mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 54, 73-96.

Green, M. C., Visser, P. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (2002). Coping with accountability cross-pressures: Low effort evasive tactics and high-effort quests for complex compromises. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 1380-1391.

Greene, D., & Lepper, M. R. (1974). Effects of extrinsic rewards on children's subsequent intrinsic interest. Child Development, 45, 1141-1145.

Greguras, G. J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2009). Different fits satisfy different needs: Linking person-environment fit to employee commitment and performance using self-determination theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 465-477.

Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children's learning: An experimental and individual difference investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 890-898.

Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated with children's self-regulation and competence in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143-154.

Gropel, P. &, Kuhl, J. (2009). Work-life balance and subjective well-being: The mediating role of need fulfillment. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 365-375.

Guillen-Royo, M., & Kasser, T. (2015). Personal goals, socio-economic context and happiness: Studying a diverse sample in Peru. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16, 405-425. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9515-6

Hackman, J.R., & Oldham, G.R. (1975). Development of the Job Diagnostic Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 159-170.

Hackman, J.R., & Oldham, G.R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 250-279.

Hall, A. T., Frink, D. D., Ferris, G. R., Hochwarter, W. A., Kacmar, C. J., & Bowen, M. G. (2003). Accountability in human resources management. In C. A. Schriesheim & L. L. Neider (Eds.), New directions in human resources management (pp. 29-63). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.

Hall, A. T., Royle, M. T., Brymer, R. A., Perrewe, G. R., Ferris, G. R., & Hochwarter, W. A. (2006). Relationships between felt accountability as a stressor and strain reactions: The neutralizing role of autonomy across two studies. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 87-99.

Harackiewicz, J. M. (1979). The effects of reward contingency and performance feedback on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1352-1363.

Hepburn, A., & Brown, S. (2001). Teacher stress and the management of accountability. Human Relations, 54, 691-715.

Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111-127.

Hodgins, H. S. (2008). Motivation, threshold for threat, and quieting the ego. In H. Wayment & J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self interest: Psychological explorations of the quiet ego (pp. 117-124). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hodgins, H. S., Brown, A. B., & Carver, B. (2007). Autonomy and control motivation and self-esteem. Self and Identity, 6, 189-208.

Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and conscious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 87-100). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Hodgins, H. S., Koestner, R., & Duncan, N. (1996). On the compatibility of autonomy and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 227-237.

Hodgins, H. S., & Liebeskind, E. (2003). Apology versus defense: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 297-316.

Hodgins, H. S., Liebeskind, E., & Schwartz, W. (1996). Getting out of hot water: Facework in social predicaments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 300-314.

Hodgins, H. S., Weibust, K. S., Weinstein, N., Shiffman, S., Miller, A., Coombs, G.,& Adair, K. C. (2010). The cost of self-protection: threat response and performance as a function of autonomous and controlled motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1101-1114.

Hodgins, H. S., Yacko, H., & Gottlieb, E. (2006). Autonomy and nondefensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 283-293.

Hoffman, B. J., & Woehr, D. J. (2006). A quantitative review of the relationship between person-organization fit and behavioral outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 389-399.

Hope, N. H., Milyavskaya, M., Holding, A. C., & Koestner, R. (2013). Self-growth in the college years: Increased importance of intrinsic values predicts resolution of identity and intimacy stages. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(6), 705-712. doi:10.1177/1948550613516875

Huang, X., Iun, J., Liu, A., & Gong, Y. (2010). Does participative leadership enhance work performance by inducing empowerment or trust? The differential effects on managerial and non-managerial subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31, 122-143.

Huang, Y., Wang, L., & Shi, J. (2009). When do objects become more attractive? The individual and interactive effects of choice and ownership on object evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 713-722.

Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Integrating motivational, social, and contextual work design features: A meta-analytic summary and theoretical extension of work design literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1332-1356.

Jansen, K. J., & Kristof-Brown, A. (2006). Toward a multidimensional theory of person-environment fit. Journal of Managerial Issues, 18, 193-212

Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kasser, T. & Sheldon, K. M. (2009). Material and time affluence as predictors of subjective well-being. Journal of Business Ethics, 84, 243-255.

Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well-being in business students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 137-146.

Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280-287.

Klar, M., & Kasser, T. (2010). Some benefits of being an activist: measuring activism and its role in psychological well-being. Psychological Science, 30, 755-777.

Koberg, C. S., Boss, R. W., Senjem, J. C., & Goodman, E. A. (1999). Antecedents and outcomes of empowerment: Empirical evidence from the health care industry. Group and Organization Management, 24, 71-91.

Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Setting limits on children's behavior: The differential effects of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic motivation and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233-248.

Kristof, A. L. (1996). Person-organization fit: An integrative review of its conceptualizations, measurement, and implications. Personnel Psychology, 49, 1-49.

Kristof-Brown, A. L., Jansen, K. J., & Colbert, A. E. (2002). A policy-capturing study of the simultaneous effects of fit with jobs, groups, and organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 985-993.

Kristof-Brown, A., Barrick, M. R., & Franke, M. (2002). Applicant impression management: Dispositional influences and consequences for recruiter perceptions of fit and similarity. Journal of Management, 28, 27-46.

Laird, M. D., Perryman, A. A., Hochwarter, W. A., Ferris, G. R., & Zinko, R. (2009). The moderating effects of personal reputation on accountability-strain relationships. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 70-83.

Larson, D. G., & Chastain, R. L. (1990). Self-concealment: Conceptualization, measurement, and health implications. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 439-455.

Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., Grant, P., & Chung, J. (2007). On the self-regulation of implicit and explicit prejudice: A self-determination theory perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 732-749.

Lekes, N., Hope, N. H., Gouvei, L., Koestner, R., & Philippe, F. L. (2012). Influencing value priorities and increasing well-being: The effects of reflecting on intrinsic values. Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 249-261.

Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (eds.) (1978). The hidden costs of reward: New perspectives on the psychology of human motivation. Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.

Liden, R., Wayne, S. J., & Sparrowe, R. T. (2000). An examination of the mediating role of psychological empowerment on the relations between the job, interpersonal relationships, and work outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 407-416.

Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum Press.

Lind, E. A., Kanfer, R., & Earley, P. C. (1990). Voice, control, and procedural justice: Instrumental and noninstrumental concerns in fairness judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 952-959.

Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., Ambrose, M., & De Vera-Park, M. V. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224-251.

Lloyd, R. (2008). Discretionary effort and the performance domain. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Organisational Psychology, 1, 22-34.

Loveland, K. K., & Olley, J. G. (1979). The effect of external reward on interest and quality of task performance in children of high and low intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 50, 1207-1210.

Lund, O. C. H., Tamnes, C. K., Moestue, C., Buss, D. M., & Vollrath, M. (2007). Tactics of hierarchy negotiation. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 25-44.

Matarazzo, K. L., Durik, A. M., & Delaney, M. L. (2010). The effect of humorous instructional materials on interest in a math task. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 293-305.

Moller, A. C., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Interpersonal control, dehumanization, and violence: A self-determination theory perspective. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13, 41-53.

Morgan, M. (1981). The overjustification effect: A developmental test of self perception interpretations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 809-821.

Morgan, M. (1983). Decrements in intrinsic motivation among rewarded and observer subjects. Child Development, 54, 636-644.

Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). A graded conceptualization of selfdetermination in the regulation of exercise behavior: Development of a measure using confirmatory factor analytic procedures. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 745-752.

Niessen, C., & Volmer, J. (2010). Adaptation to increased work autonomy: The role of task reflection. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 19, 442-460.

Orpen, C. (2000). The interactive effects of role uncertainty and accountability on employees use of upward influence tactics. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 37, 2-4.

Ostroff, C., & Schulte, M. (2007). Multiple perspectives of fit in organizations across levels of analysis. [References]. In C. Ostroff & T. A. Judge (Eds.), Perspectives on organizational fit (pp. 3-69). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Ostroff, C., Shin, Y., & Kinicki, A. J. (2005). Multiple perspectives of congruence: Relationships between value congruence and employee attitudes. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 591-623.

Page, S. (2006). The web of managerial accountability: The impact of reinventing government. Administration & Society, 38, 166-197.

Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-D'Angelo, M., & Reid, R. (2004). Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationships between forms of regulation, eating behaviors, sustained dietary behavior change and psychological adjustment. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 245-277.

Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Briere, N. M. (2001). Associations among perceived autonomy support, forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospective study. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279-306.

Perry, D. G., Bussey, K., & Redman, J. (1977). Reward-induced decreased play effects: Reattribution of motivation, competing responses, or avoid frustration? Child Development, 48, 1369-1374.

Piasentin, K. A., & Chapman, D. S. (2007). Perceived similarity and complementarity as predictors of subjective person-organization fit. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80, 341-354.

Pieterse, A. N., Van Knippenberg, D., Schippers, M., & Stam, D. (2010). Transformational and transactional leadership and innovative behavior: The moderating role of psychological empowerment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31, 609-623.

Plant, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and the effects of self-consciousness, self-awareness, and ego-involvement: An investigation of internally controlling styles. Journal of Personality, 53, 435-449.

Quinn, R. E., & Spreitzer, G. M. (1997). The road to empowerment: seven questions every leader should consider. Organizational Dynamics, 26, 37-49.

Quoidbach, Dunn, E. W., Petrides, K. V., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Money giveth, money taketh away: The dual effect of wealth on happiness. Psychological Science, 21, 759-763.

Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, R., & Ryan, R. (2000). Daily well being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419-435.

Riketta, M., & Landerer, A. (2002). Organizational commitment, accountability, and work behavior: A correlational study. Social Behavior and Personality, 30, 653-660.

Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands people's perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychological science, 23, 1130-1136. doi:10.1177/0956797612438731

Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450-461.

Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749-761.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78.

Ryan, R. M., & Lynch, J. (1989). Emotional autonomy versus detachment: Revisiting the vicissitudes of adolescence and young adulthood. Child Development, 60, 340-356.

Ryan, R. M., & Solky, J. A. (1996). What is supportive about social support? On the psychological needs for autonomy and relatedness. In G.R. Pierce & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Handbook of social support and the family: Plenum series on stress and coping (pp. 249-267). New York: Plenum.

Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Ego-involved persistence: When freechoice behavior is not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 185-205.

Ryan, R. M., Mims, V., & Koestner, R. (1983). Relation of reward contingency and interpersonal context to intrinsic motivation: A review and test using cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 736-750.

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of religious internalization and their relations to religious orientations and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 586-596.

Sakalaki, M., & Fousiani, K. (2011). About some personality misfortunes of opportunists: The negative correlation of economic defection with autonomy, agreeableness, and well-being. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 471-487. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00780.x

Schlegel, R. J., Hicks, J. A., Arndt, J., & King, L. A. (2009). Thine own self: True self-concept accessibility and meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 473-490.

Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and self-determination in personality. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 61-72.

Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Assessing the sustainability of goal-based changes in well-being over a four-year period. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 223-229.

Sheldon, K. M. & Bettencourt, B. A. (2002). Psychological needs and subjective well-being in social groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 25-38.

Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are "personal": Comparing autonomous and controlling goals on effort and attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546-557.

Sheldon, K. M., & Filak, V. (2008). Manipulating autonomy, competence, and relatedness in a game-learning context: New evidence that all three needs matter. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 267-283.

Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546-557.

Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2008). Psychological threat and goal striving. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 37-45.

Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. (1996). What makes for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270-1279.

Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It's both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475-486.

Sheldon, K., Williams, G. C., & Joiner, T. (2003). Self-determination theory in the clinic: Motivating physical and mental health. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Siegall, M., & Gardner, S. (2000). Contextual factors of psychological empowerment. Personnel Review, 29, 703-722.

Siegel-Jacobs, K., & Yates, J. F. (1996). Effects of procedural and outcome accountability on judgment quality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 1-17.

Silvia, P. J. (2003). Self-efficacy and interest: Experimental studies of optimal incompetence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 237-249.

Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89-102.

Sims Jr., H.P., Szilagyi, A.D., & Keller, R.T. (1976). The measurement of job characteristics. Academy of Management Journal, 19, 195-211.

Sims, R. L., & Keon, T. L. (1997). Ethical work climate as a factor in the development of person-organization fit. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 1095-1105.

Spreitzer, G. M. (1995). Individual empowerment in the workplace: Dimensions, measurement, and validation. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 1442-1465.

Spreitzer, G. M. (1996). Social structural characteristics of psychological empowerment. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 483-504.

Spreitzer, G. M., Kizilos, M. A., & Nason, S. W. (1997). A multidimensional analysis of the relationship between psychological empowerment, and effectiveness, satisfaction, and strain. Journal of Management, 23, 679-705.

Tan, H., & Tan, C. (2002). Temporary employees in Singapore. What drives them? Journal of Psychology, 136, 83-102.

Tang, S., & Hall, V. C. (1995). The overjustification effect: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 365-404.

Tetlock, P. (1983). Accountability and the perseverance of first impressions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 74-83.

Tetlock, P. (1985). Accountability: The neglected social context of judgment and choice. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 297-332). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Thomas, K. W., & Tymon, W. G., Jr. (1994), Does empowerment always work: Understanding the role of intrinsic motivation and personal interpretation. Journal of Management Systems, 6, 1-13.

Thomas, K. W., & Velthouse, B. A. (1990). Cognitive elements of empowerment: an 'interpretive' model of intrinsic task motivation. Academy of Management Review, 15, 666-681.

Thoms, P., Dose, J. J., & Scott, K. S. (2002). Relationships between accountability, job satisfaction, and trust. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 13, 307-323.

Uysal, A., Lin, H. L., & Knee, C. R. (2009). The role of need satisfaction in self concealment and wellbeing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 187-199.

Vallerand, R. J. , Blanchard, C. M. , Mageau, G. A. , Koestner, R., Ratelle, C. F., Lonard, M., Marsolais, J. et al. (2003). Les passions de l'me: On obsessive and harmonious passion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 ,756-767.

Van den Bos, K. (2001). Uncertainty management: The influence of human uncertainty on reactions to perceived fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 931-941.

Van den Bos, K. (2003). On the subjective quality of social justice: The role of affect as information in the psychology of justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 482-498.

Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means of fairness judgments. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 1-60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Van den Bos, K., & Van Prooijen, J.-W. (2001). Referent cognitions theory: The psychology of voice depends on closeness of reference points. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 616-626.

Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. A. M., & Lind, E. A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449-1458.

van Prooijen, J. (2009). Procedural justice as autonomy regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1166-1180.

Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19-31.

Vansteenkiste, M., Neyrinck, B., Niemiec, C. P., Soenens, B., De Witte, H., & Van den Broeck, A. (2007). On the relations among work value orientations, psychological need satisfaction, and job outcomes: A self-determination theory approach. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80, 251-277.

Vansteenkiste, M., Niemiec, C., & Soenens, B. (2010). The development of the five mini-theories of self-determination theory: An historical overview, emerging trends, and future directions. In T. Urdan & S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement. The decade ahead (Vol. 16, pp. 105-166). UK: Emerald Publishing

Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivation learning, performance and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246-260.

Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Matos, L. (2005). Examining the impact of extrinsic versus intrinsic goal framing and internally controlling versus autonomy-supportive communication style on early adolescents' academic achievement. Child Development, 76, 483-501.

Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Soenens, B., & Lens, W. (2004). How to become a persevering exerciser? Providing a clear, future intrinsic goal in an autonomy supportive way. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 232-249.

Verquer, M. L., Beehr, T. A., & Wagner, S. H. (2003). A meta-analysis of relations between person-organization fit and work attitudes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 63, 473-489.

Weinstein, N., & Hodgins, H. S. (2009). The moderating role of autonomy and control on the benefits of written emotion expression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 351-364.

Weinstein, N., Przybylski, A. K., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Can nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1315-1329.

Wiechman, B. M., & Gurland, S. T. (2009). What happens during the free-choice period? Evidence of a polarizing effect of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 716-719.

Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychosocial values by medical students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 767-779.

Williams, G. C., Cox, E., Kouides, R., & Deci, E. L. (1999). Presenting the facts about smoking to adolescents: The effects of an autonomy supportive style. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 153, 959-964.

Williams, G. C., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (1998). Supporting autonomy to motivate patients with diabetes for glucose control. Diabetes Care, 21, 1644-1651.

Williams, G. C., Gagne, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). Facilitating autonomous motivation for smoking cessation. Health Psychology, 21, 40-50.

Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and weight loss maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 115-126.

Williams, G. C., Levesque, C. S., Zeldman, A., Wright, S., & Deci,E. L. (2003). Health care practitioners' motivation for tobacco dependence counseling. Health Education Research, 18, 538-553.

Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Sharp, D., Levesque, C., Kouides, R. W., & Ryan, R. M.et al. (2006). Testing a self-determination theory intervention for motivating tobacco cessation: Supporting autonomy and competence in a clinical trial. Health Psychology, 25, 95-101.

Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Zeldman, A., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Testing a self-determination theory process model for promoting glycemic control through diabetes self-management. Health Psychology, 23, 58-66.

Williams, G. C., Minicucci, D. M., Kouides, R. M., Levesque, C. S., Chirkov, V. I., & Ryan, R. M.et al. (2002). Self-determination, smoking, diet, and health. Health Education Research, 17, 512-521.

Williams, G. C., Rodin, G. C., Ryan, R. M., Grolnick, W. S., & Deci, E. L. (1998). Autonomous regulation and long-term medication adherence in adult outpatients. Health Psychology, 17, 269-276.



Academic Scholar?
Join our team of writers.
Write a new opinion article,
a new Psyhclopedia article review
or update a current article.
Get recognition for it.





Last Update: 7/20/2016