Tipultech logo

Opponent process theory

Author: Dr Simon Moss

Overview

A variety of studies show that mood states and satisfaction tend to be relatively stable across time. Furthermore, mood and satisfaction do not differ appreciably across contexts or cultures. To illustrate, many studies indicate that job satisfaction does not vary considerably over time. A meta-analysis showed that measures of job satisfaction at two distinct times, on average separated by three years, generated a correlation of .42 (see Dormann & Zapf, 2001).

A variety of explanations have been posited to explain the uniformity of mood or satisfaction across time or contexts. These theories explain that many attempts to enhance mood or increase job satisfaction, for example, typically fail. The opponent process theory was formulated to explain these patterns of observations (Landy, 1978;; Solomon & Corbit, 1973, 1974).

Description of the model

According to opponent process theory (Bowling, Beehr, Wagner, & Libkuman, 2005;; Solomon & Corbit, 1973, 1974), emotional events, such as criticisms or rewards, elicit two sets of processes. The first set is called primary processes, which evoke emotions that are consistent with the affective tone of the event. Threatening events, such as examinations, elicit processes that provoke anxiety. Relaxing events, such as meditation, evoke processes that foster composure.

The second set is opponent processes, which evoke emotions that counter these primary processes. Threatening events elicit opponent processes that promote composure. Relaxing events evoke opponent processes that provoke anxiety, and so forth.

Several properties differentiate primary and opponent processes. First, most emotional events will elicit primary processes. Only the more intense events will elicit opponent processes. Specifically, when the intensity or magnitude of the primary processes exceeds some threshold, opponent processes are evoked.

Second, the primary process is elicited rapidly but also declines quickly. The opponent process, in contrast, is evoked more slowly but also wanes gradually. As a consequence, in response to aversive events, individuals often experience unpleasant emotions initially that gradually decline. Indeed, at some point in the trajectory, the opponent processes often overrides the primary process, and a pleasant emotion is experienced--called an overshoot effect. Positive events can elicit the converse trajectory of emotions.

Third, frequent exposure to a specific event, such as examinations or meditation, does not significantly affect the magnitude or trajectory of primary processes. After regular exposure to these events, however, the opponent process is evoked more rapidly and diminishes more slowly.

Implications and evidence of opponent process theory

The emotional response of parachute jumpers, characterized by Epstein (1967), can be ascribed to opponent process theory (Bowling, Beehr, Wagner, & Libkuman, 2005). In particular, when novices jump, they initially experience intense negative emotions, representing primary processes, followed by negligible, if any, positive emotions, representing limited opponent processes. Experienced jumpers, however, experience less intense negative emotions followed by pronounced positive states. That is, with experience, the opponent processes, which foster positive states, are elicited rapidly and maintained appreciably.

Opponent processes theory has been substantiated in many domains. The theory has been tested in the context of opiate use, romantic love, and electric shocks (see Ettenberg, Raven, Danluck, & Necessary, 1999;; Hoffman & Solomon, 1974;; Ley, 1994;; Myers & Siegel, 1985). In all of these instances, over time, aversive events eventually become more likely to elicit positive emotions. In addition, desirable events, over time, become more likely to evoke negative emotions.

Similarly, many studies have shown that emotional responses to changes in the work context are often transient, consistent with opponent process theory. As Williams, Suls, Alliger, Learner, and Wan (1991) showed, conflicting roles at work initially evoke negative emotions--emotions that gradually abate over weeks.

Mechanisms that underpin opponent processes

The mechanisms that underpin opponent processes have not been characterized definitively. Potentially, opponent processes evoked by adverse events and opponent processes evoked by positive event might correspond to distinct mechanisms. For example, opponent processes elicited by adverse events might represent forms of emotional regulation that become automatic over time (Bargh & Williams, 2007). After an adversity, such as a failure, individuals might attempt to focus on the positive implications of this episode. Repeated attempts to focus on positive implications might become automatic with time and thus might be elicited more rapidly and persist effortlessly (for another possible mechanism, see physiological toughness).

Opponent processes evoked by positive events, however, might evolve from other mechanisms. Individuals might learn that desirable events do not always culminate in persistent positive emotions, because other complexities often intervene (see Affective forecasting). Over time, they might associate positive events with negative consequences.

Associations with alternative theories

Disposition theories

To explain the stability of affect and satisfaction, many other theories or perspectives have been proposed. One explanation is that affect or satisfaction represents enduring traits, sometimes governed by genetics.

To illustrate, this explanation has often been applied to explain the stability of job satisfaction across times. Ilies and Judge (2003), for example, showed that genetics can accommodate variations in dispositions, such as personality, which in turn govern job satisfaction. Specifically, individuals who exhibit extraversion tend to experience positive affect (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988;; see Five factor model), which tends to be highly related to job satisfaction (Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000). Individuals who exhibit neuroticism tend to experience negative affect (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), which in turn is associated with job dissatisfaction (Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000).

Similarly, Judge and Bono (2001) reveal that job satisfaction is related to a broad trait called core self evaluations. Core self evaluations entail self esteem, generalized self efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability--the converse of neuroticism (Bono & Judge, 2003;; Erez & Judge, 2001). These four traits are highly related to each other and do not show discriminant validity (Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen, 2002).

Nevertheless, according to Bowling, Beehr, Wagner, and Libkuman (2005), this perspective does not contradict opponent process theory. If individuals exhibit elevated levels of extraversion, the threshold to evoke opponent processes in response to positive events might be elevated, and these processes might dissipate rapidly. Similarly, if individuals exhibit neuroticism, the threshold to elicit opponent processes in response to negative events might be elevated. In other words, dispositions might affect thresholds or trajectories of opponent processes.

Set point theory

Set point theory, like dispositional theories and opponent process theory, also predicts that wellbeing tends to fluctuate around a stable level--a level that generally remains uniform over time (for a discussion, see Fujita & Diener, 2005). After individuals experience positive events, their wellbeing might rise transiently but then revert to this stable level or set point. Likewise, after individuals experience negative events, their wellbeing might decline momentarily, but will then regress to the previous level.

To some extent, set point theory emanated from seminal research, conducted by Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman (1978). This study compared the happiness of 22 people who had won lotteries with 29 people who had experienced paralysis as a consequence of accidents. After some delay, the happiness of these individuals did not differ significantly from the happiness of a control group of participants. The implication of this study is that, after a pivotal event, the happiness of individuals changes temporarily but then returns to the previous level.

To explain this stability of wellbeing over time, proponent of set point theory maintain that affective states are primarily dependent upon enduring dispositions (see Huppert, 2005). Personality traits, for example, are assumed to determine the emotional experiences and responses of individuals. A meta-analysis of 148 studies, conducted by DeNeve and Cooper (1998), showed that neuroticism is inversely related to subjective wellbeing--which represents a combination of positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction (see Diener, 2000)& the correlation was -.22. Furthermore, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and extraversion were positively related to subjective wellbeing, generating correlations of .21, .17, and .17 respectively. Overall, Steel, Schmidt, and Schulz (2008) showed that 39% to 63% of the variance in subjective wellbeing can be ascribed to personality.

Adaptation level theory

Another framework, which can explain the stability of mood and satisfaction, is adaptation level theory (Helson, 1948, 1964). According to this theory, over time, individuals form expectations of the future, called frames of reference. Events that are more favorable than such expectations evoke positive emotions, whereas events that are less favorable than such expectations evoke negative emotions.

These events, however, also shape the expectations or frames of reference. To illustrate, if individuals are assigned to a salubrious office--an office that exceeds their expectations--they will initially enjoy positive emotions. Over time, however, their frame of references changes and they expect these surroundings. Hence, after several weeks or months, the office no longer elicits positive affective states.

Several scholars, however, argue that adaptation level theory does not contradict opponent process theory and, indeed, these frameworks can be integrated (e.g., Bowling, Beehr, Wagner, & Libkuman, 2005;; Oliver, 1981). Adaptation level theory emphasizes that previous experience can affect responses to emotional events. These previous experiences might primarily shape the opponent processes, which in turn determines the aggregate responses to emotional events.

Factors that moderate adaptation or satiation

Over time, the extent to which individuals enjoy a specific product, activity, or person, such as a song, a meal, or a friend, tends to dissipate upon repeated exposure--called satiation, and this satiation might partly underpin adaptation. After listening to a song once, individuals might enjoy the music considerably. After listening to the song many times, however, this enjoyment tends to wane. Many objects or activities that evoke pleasure might become less effective over time, limiting the mood and wellbeing of individuals.

Galak, Redden, and Kruger (2009), however, uncovered a mental exercise that curbs this diminution in enjoyment. In particular, individuals are merely instructed to reflect upon a broader variety of similar products, activities, or people to which they have been recently exposed. To illustrate, in a pilot study, individuals were asked to specify the person with whom they interacted most during the past two weeks, representing possible satiation. They were also asked to specify the extent to which they would enjoy interacting with this person again soon.

Interspersed between these two questions, some participants were then asked to list all the other friends to whom they interacted during this period. Other participants, assigned to the control conditions, were instructed either to list the celebrities they had observed on the news during this time or friends to whom they might interact in the next two weeks.

Relative to the other conditions, if participants had reflected upon other friends to whom they had interacted over the last two weeks, they were more willing to socialize with the friend to whom they interacted the most. Interactions with this friend did not seem as monotonous.

According to Galak, Redden, and Kruger (2009), after participants listed the other friends with whom they interacted during the previous fortnight, they perceived the variety of these interactions as acceptable. They did not, therefore, need to seek more variety in this domain. Satiation, which arguably reflects a need to seek variety, was thus curbed.

Additional studies confirm this argument. In one study, participants listened to the chorus of one song they like 20 times consecutively and the chorus of another song they like once. After each occasion, they rated the extent to which they enjoyed the song. Repeated exposure to one song elicited satiation: They become less inclined to like the song over time.

Three weeks later, some participants were asked to recall all the musicians they have heard in the last few weeks. Other participants, assigned to the control condition, were asked to recall all the TV shows they had watched during this time. Finally, participants heard the two songs again, evaluating the extent to which they enjoyed this music.

After individuals reflected upon all the musicians, rather than TV shows, to which they had been exposed, satiation diminished. That is, these individuals still enjoyed the chorus they had, three weeks earlier, listened to 20 times. Somehow, these reflections implied they had listened to a sufficient variety of music. They did not, thus, need to seek more variety. They were therefore willing to hear a song they had listened to repeatedly before.

One alternative explanation emerges from a key feature of the method: The music that had been presented repeatedly was one of the favorite songs of participants. The gamut of songs they heard over the last few weeks may not have been as desirable. The favorite song of participants might seem especially enjoyable after they reflect upon undesirable music, referred to as evaluative contrast.

The final study, conducted by Galak, Redden, and Kruger (2009), challenges this explanation. In this study, participants consumed one color of jelly beans many times. They then consumed a sample of different colors. The color that participants consumed many times was perceived as more enjoyable if these individuals were instructed to write a few sentences about the sample of different colors rather than write a few sentences about the one color. Even if participants did not especially like this color--and preferred the colors in the other sample--this pattern of observations was observed.

In short, as Galak, Redden, and Kruger (2009) emphasized, memories of previous variety reduce satiation. This effect does not emerge, however, if participants reflect upon an unrelated product or activity. Furthermore, this effect does not emerge if participants imagine they will be exposed to variety in the future.

One of the implications of this finding is that individuals should reflect upon the variety of activities they experience, curbing satiation and, thus, potentially improving wellbeing. In addition, advertisers should incorporate a variety of related products in the commercials, perhaps incidentally, to reduce satiation towards the target brand.

Practical implications

Opponent process theory generates many practical implications. To illustrate, when managers introduce initiatives, intended to evoke positive emotions, opponent processes might eventually prevail and negative feelings might ensue. To preclude these negative emotions, several measures should be considered. First, initiatives that evoke mild, rather than pronounced, positive emotions should be preferred--these initiatives are less likely to elicit opponent processes. Second, initiatives should be modified over time& initiatives that do not change are more likely to elicit opponent processes.

References

Alesina, A., Di Tella, R., & MacCulloch, R. J. (2004). Inequality and happiness: Are Europeans and Americans different? Journal of Public Economics, 88, 2009-2042.

Arvey, R. D., McCall, B. P., Bouchard, T. J., Taubman, P., & Cavanaugh, M. A. (1994). Genetic influences on job satisfaction and work values. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 21-33.

Bargh, J. A., & Williams, L. E. (2007). The nonconscious regulation of emotion. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 429-445). New York: Guildford.

Biswas-Diener, R., Vitters?, J., & Diener, E. (2005). Most people are pretty happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, the Amish, and the Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies: An Interdisciplinary Periodical on Subjective Well-Being, 6, 205-226.

Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., & Schilling, E. A. (1989). Effects of daily stress on negative mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 808-818.

Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Core self-evaluations: A review of the trait and its role in job satisfaction and performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, S5-S18.

Bowling, N. A., Beehr, T. A., Wagner, S. H., & Libkuman, T. M. (2005). Adaptation-level theory, opponent process theory, and dispositions: An integrated approach to the stability of job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1044-1053.

Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917-927.

Buessing, A., Bissels, T., Fuchs, V., & Perrar, K.-M. (1999). A dynamic model of work satisfaction: Qualitative approaches. Human Relations, 52, 999-1028.

Cole, R. P., Shakespeare, V., Shakespeare, P.G., & Hobby, J. A. E. (1994). Measuring outcomes in low-priority plastic surgery patients using Quality of Life indices. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 47, 117-121.

Connolly, J. J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2000). The role of affectivity in job satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 265-281.

DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1988). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197-229.

Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness, and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, 34-43.

Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? A literature review and guide to needed research. Social Indicators Research, 57, 119-169.

Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Rethinking happiness: The science of psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105-1117.

Diener, E., & Lucas, R. (1999). Personality, and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213-229). New York: Sage.

Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 185-218). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 1-31.

Diener, E., & Tov, W. (2007). Subjective well-being and peace. Journal of Social Issues, 63, 421-440.

Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 851-864.

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75.

Diener, E., Gohm, C., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 419-436.

Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., Oishi, S., & Suh, E. M. (2002). Looking up and looking down: Weighting good and bad information in life satisfaction judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 437-445.

Diener, E., Lucas, R., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61, 305-314.

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403-425.

Diener, E., Scollon, C. K. N., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Suh, E. M. (2000). Positivity and the construction of life satisfaction judgments: Global happiness is not the sum of its parts. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1, 159-176.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276-302.

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., & Oishi, S. (2001). End effects of rated life quality: The James Dean effect. Psychological Science, 12, 124-128.

Dormann, C., & Zapf, D. (2001). Job satisfaction: A meta-analysis of stabilities. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 483-504.

Erez, A., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations to goal setting, motivation, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 1270-1279.

Ettenberg, A., Raven, M. A., Danluck, D. A., & Necessary, B. D. (1999). Evidence for opponent-process actions of intravenous cocaine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 64, 507-512.

Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundation of hedonic psychology (pp. 302-329). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set-point: Stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 158-164.

Galak, J., Redden, J. P., & Kruger, J. (2009). Variety amnesia: Recalling past variety can accelerate recovery from satiation. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 575-584.

Gerhart, B. (1987). How important are dispositional factors as determinants of job satisfaction? Implications for job design and other personnel programs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 366-377.

Helson, H. (1948). Adaptation-level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference. Psychological Review, 55, 297-313.

Helson, H. (1964). Current trends and issues in adaptation-level theory. American Psychologist, 19, 26-38.

Hoffman, H. S., & Solomon, R. L. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: III. Some affective dynamics in imprinting. Learning and Motivation, 5, 149-164.

Huppert, F. A. (2005). Positive mental health in individuals and populations. In F. A., Huppert, N. Baylis, & B. Kenerne (Eds.), The science of wellbeing (pp. 307-340). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2003). On the heritability of job satisfaction: The mediating role of personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 750-759.

Isen, A. M., Clark, M., & Schwartz, M. F. (1976). Duration of the effect of good mood on helping: "Footprints on the sands of time." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 385-393.

Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits-self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability-with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 80-92.

Judge, T. A., & Larsen, R. J. (2001). Dispositional affect and job satisfaction: A review and theoretical extension. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 67-98.

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., & Locke, E. A. (2000). Personality and job satisfaction: The mediating role of job characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 237-249.

Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core construct? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 693-710.

Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. (2002). Five-factor model of personality and job satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 530-541.

Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. (1997). The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A core evaluations approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19, 151-188.

Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., Durham, C. C., & Kluger, A. N. (1998). Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 17-34.

Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (2003). Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Landy, F. J. (1978). An opponent process theory of job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 533-547.

Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129-141.

Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132-140.

Ley, R. (1994). An opponent-process interpretation of the anxiolytic effects of single inhalations of large concentrations of carbon dioxide. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 301-309.

Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527-539.

Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set-point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 8-13.

Lucas, R. E., Dyrenforth, P. S., & Diener, E. (2008). Four myths about subjective well-being. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 2001-2015.

Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness as a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186-189.

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803-855.

Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion and neuroticism as predictors of objective life events: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1046-1053.

McNiel, J. M., & Fleeson, W. (2006). The causal effects of extraversion on positive affect and neuroticism on negative affect: Manipulating state extraversion and state neuroticism in an experimental approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 529-550.

Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1996). The pursuit of happiness. Scientific American, 70-72.

Necowitz, L. B., & Roznowski, M. (1994). Negative affectivity and job satisfaction: Cognitive processes underlying the relationship and effects on employee behaviors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45, 270-294.

Ng, W. & Diener, E. (2009). Personality differences in emotions: Does emotion regulation play a role? Journal of Individual Differences, 30, 100-106.

Ng, W., & Diener, E. (2009). Feeling bad? The "power" of positive thinking may not apply to everyone. Journal of Research and Personality, 43, 455-463.

Ng, W., Diener, E., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2009). Affluence, feelings of stress, and well-being. Social Indicators Research, 94, 257-271.

Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Re-examining the general positivity model of subjective well-being: The discrepancy between specific and global domain satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 69, 641-666.

Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R.E. (2007). Optimal level of well-being: Can people be too happy?. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346-360.

Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D. W., Kim-Prieto, C., & Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 685-698.

Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., Diener, E., Kim-Prieto, C., Scollon, C. N., Choi, D. (2007). The value-congruence model of memory for emotional experiences: An explanation for cultural and individual differences in emotional self-reports. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 897-905.

Pacek, A., & Radcliff, B. (2008). Assessing the welfare state: The politics of happiness. Perspectives on Politics, 6, 267-277.

Radcliff, B. (2001). Politics, markets, and life satisfaction: The political economy of human happiness. American Political Science Review, 95, 939-952.

Roysamb, E., Tambs, K., Reichborn,-Kjennerud, T., Neale, M. C., & Harris, J. R. (2003). Happiness and health: Environmental and genetic contributions to the relationship between subjective well-being, perceived health, and somatic illness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1136-1146.

Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1152-1165.

Smith, R. H., Parrott, W. G., Diener, E., Hoyle, R. H., & Kim, S.H. (1999). Dispositional envy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1007-1020.

Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1973). An opponent-process theory of motivation: II. Cigarette addiction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, 158-171.

Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119-145.

Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Updegraff, J. A. (2008). From culture to priming conditions - Self-construal influences on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 39, 3-15.

Suls, J., Green, P., & Hillis, S. (1998). Emotional reactivity to everyday problems, affective inertia, and neuroticism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 127-136.

Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A. P., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 914-945.

Veenhoven, R. (2000). Well-being in the welfare state: Level not higher, distribution not more equitable. Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis, 2, 91-125.

Wassamer, R. W., Jr, Lascher, E. L., & Kroll, S. (2009). Sub-national fiscal activity as a determinant of individual happiness: Ideology matters. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 563-582.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465-490.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.

Watson, D., Pennebaker, J. W., & Folger, R. (1986). Beyond negative affectivity: Measuring stress and satisfaction in the workplace. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 8, 141-157.

Weiss, A., Bates, T. C., & Luciano, M. (2008). Happiness is a personal(ity) thing: The genetics of personality and subjective well-being in a representative sample. Psychological Science, 19, 205-210.

Weiss, H. M., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. S. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in affective experiences over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 1-24.

Williams, D. E., & Thompson, J. K. (1993). A set-point hypothesis of psychological functioning. Behavior Modification, 17, 43-57.

Williams, K. J., Suls, J., Alliger, G. M., Learner, S. M., & Wan, C. K. (1991). Multiple role juggling and daily mood states in working mothers: An experience sampling study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 664-674.

Wirtz, D., Chiu, C., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2009). What constitutes a good life? Cultural differences in the role of positive and negative affect in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 77, 1167-1196.



Academic Scholar?
Join our team of writers.
Write a new opinion article,
a new Psyhclopedia article review
or update a current article.
Get recognition for it.





Last Update: 6/30/2016